[1]韩济生. 神经科学. 第三版[M]. 北京:北京大学医学出版社, 2009, 636.
[2]罗艳琳, 李俊发, 陈昭燃. 疼痛注意研究的实验范式[J]. 重庆医学杂志, 2010,21:3125-3129.
[3]Macleod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Buletin[J], 1991, 109: 163-203.
[4] Edwards MS, Burt JS, Lipp OV. Selective attention for masked and unmasked threatening words in anxiety: Effects of trait anxiety, state anxiety and awareness[J]. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2010, 48: 210–218.
[5] Kertzman S, Reznik I, Hornik L. T, et al. Stroop performance in major depression: Selective attention impairment or psychomotor slowness? [J]. J Affective Disorders, 2010, 122: 167–173.
[6]Gijsen CP, Dijkstra JB, Boxtel MPJ. Recurrent pain is associated with decreased selective attention in a population-based sample[J]. Pain, 2011, 152: 188–193.
[7]Asmundson GJG, Wright KD, Hadjistavropoulos HD. Hypervigilance and attentional fixedness in chronic musculoskeletal pain: consistency of findings across modified Stroop and dot-probe tasks[J]. J Pain, 2005, 6: 497-506.
[8] Dehghani M, Sharpe L, Nicholas MK. Selective attention to pain-related information in chronic musculoskeletal pain patients[J]. Pain, 2003, 105: 37–46.
[9]Dehghani M, Sharpe L, Nicholas MK. Modification of attentional biases in chronic pain patients: a preliminary study[J]. Euro J Pain, 2004, 8: 585–594.
[10]Baars AE, Boxtel PJ, Jolles J. Migraine does not affect cognitive decline: results from the maastricht aging study[J]. Headache, 2010, 50: 176-184.
[11] Badzakova TG, Barnett KJ, Waldie KE, et al. An ERP investigation of the Stroop task: the role of the cingulate in attentional allocation and conflict resolution[J]. Brain Search, 2009, 1253: 139–148.
[12]Roelofs J, Peters L, Zeegers P. A, et al. The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain-related stimuli among chronic pain patients a meta-analysis[J]. Eur J Pain, 2002, 6: 273-281.
[13]王海龙, 陈昭然. 止血带加压性疼痛导致交感神经兴奋性的改变[J]. 中国疼痛医学杂志, 2010, 16(2) : 76-79.
[14]Jansson B, Lundh LG, Oldenburg C. Is defensiveness associated with cognitive bias away from emotional information? [J]. Personality and Individual Differences, 2005, 39: 1373–1382.
[15] Roelofs J, Peters ML, Zeegers MPA, et al. Selective attention and avoidance of pain-related stimuli: a dot-probe evaluation in a pain-free population[J]. J Pain, 2003, 4: 322-328.
[16] Roelofs J, Peters ML, Vlaeyen JWS. Selective attention for pain-related information in healthy individuals: the role of pain and fear[J]. Eur J Pain, 2002, 6: 331–339.
[17]Asmundson GJG, Kuperosb JL, Norton GR. Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information: preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear[J]. Pain, 1997, 72: 27-32.
[18] Liossi C, White P, Schoth DE. Time-course of attentional bias for threat-related cues in patients with chronic daily headache–tension type: evidence for the role of anger. Eur J Pain 2011;15:92–98
[19] Asmundson GJG, Hadjistavropolus HD. Is high fear of pain associated with attentional biases for pain-related or general threat? A categorical reanalysis. J Pain, 2007;8:11–18.
[20]李靖, 钱秀莹. 7~11岁儿童持续注意能力的性别差异研究[J]. 应用心理学,2000, 6 : 25-28.
[21]王李艳, 隋光远, 王永辉. 视觉选择性注意中性别差异的眼动研究[J]. 心理科学, 2010, 33: 409-411. |